Research Explores Simultaneous Responses (Science Is Lovable 19 of 72)

Research Explores Simultaneous Responses (Science Is Lovable 19 of 72)

The lower pen, which we call the event pen, records the onset and offset of experiment–defined events by going up one step after which it can only go back down one step. As the paper unrolls, this pen marks a line at two slightly different levels according to whether it has moved up or moved down its one allowed step.

Past Researchers Required Single-File Responses (Science Is Lovable 18 of 72)

Past Researchers Required Single-File Responses (Science Is Lovable 18 of 72)

Given a whole, functioning physiology—an organism—behavior occurs, because eliciting, evocative, or other stimuli occur. The functionally related combination of behaviors and stimuli makes up contingencies. Occasionally a contingency occurs alone, but more often two or more contingencies operate at the same time, and researchers have called these concurrent contingencies.

A Multiple Baseline Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 17 of 72)

A Multiple Baseline Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 17 of 72)

In considering multiple–baseline designs, an example from an actual study using one of the multiple–baseline forms would help. The value of this example, which involves the behavior–based form, also resides in two of its other characteristics.

Multiple–Baseline Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 16 of 72)

Multiple–Baseline Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 16 of 72)

As early behaviorological practitioners adapted ABAB/Reversal designs to applied settings, they faced increasingly complex applied situations requiring more sophisticated single–subject experimental designs. For example, sometimes the repetition of Baseline or Intervention phases would stand out as unethical or otherwise not possible.

A Reversal Design Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 15 of 72)

A Reversal Design Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 15 of 72)

In 1975 David Anderson and I worked on an applied study at the University of Queensland in Brisbane, Australia, which was when and where I began my college teaching career. This study contains some uncommon features worthy of attention, features that one would usually only find spread across several studies.

Reversal Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 14 of 72)

Reversal Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 14 of 72)

After considering the ABAB/Reversal type of single–subject designs for basic experimental research in a recent column, this column turns to similar designs for applied research and practice. In applied settings, research considerations differ from laboratory settings, and some compel the adoption of the more complicated “multiple–baseline” single–subject designs that a later column covers.

Methodology Addresses Variability and Confidence (Science Is Lovable 13 of 72)

Methodology Addresses Variability and Confidence (Science Is Lovable 13 of 72)

The first several decades of experimental behavior science occurred from the 1930s through the 1950s. During this time Skinner, his colleagues in the natural science of behavior, and their students (i.e., the predecessors of today’s behaviorologists) gradually built the foundations of their behaviorological science mostly through the kinds of basic laboratory methodologies that we covered in the last column.

Reversal Designs Serve as Laboratory Methods (Science Is Lovable 12 of 72)

Reversal Designs Serve as Laboratory Methods (Science Is Lovable 12 of 72)

Disciplinary contingencies compelled the use of single–subject experimental designs throughout the history of natural behavior science. This started with the experimental preparations of Skinner’s early laboratory at Harvard in the 1930s and continues up through today’s laboratory and applied research efforts.

Tools and Methods Tackle Behavior Laws (Science Is Lovable 10 of 72)

Tools and Methods Tackle Behavior Laws (Science Is Lovable 10 of 72)

So, how does the natural science of behavior make discoveries? How does it apply those discoveries to bring practical benefits to people? What experimental equipment and methods does it employ in its basic and applied research? The next several columns consider the answers to these questions in terms of the standard research equipment and procedures of both the classic behavior–research laboratory and the practical research for applied settings and interventions.