A Multiple Baseline Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 17 of 72)

A Multiple Baseline Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 17 of 72)

In considering multiple–baseline designs, an example from an actual study using one of the multiple–baseline forms would help. The value of this example, which involves the behavior–based form, also resides in two of its other characteristics.

Multiple–Baseline Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 16 of 72)

Multiple–Baseline Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 16 of 72)

As early behaviorological practitioners adapted ABAB/Reversal designs to applied settings, they faced increasingly complex applied situations requiring more sophisticated single–subject experimental designs. For example, sometimes the repetition of Baseline or Intervention phases would stand out as unethical or otherwise not possible.

A Reversal Design Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 15 of 72)

A Reversal Design Deals With the Brat-Syndrome (Science Is Lovable 15 of 72)

In 1975 David Anderson and I worked on an applied study at the University of Queensland in Brisbane, Australia, which was when and where I began my college teaching career. This study contains some uncommon features worthy of attention, features that one would usually only find spread across several studies.

Reversal Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 14 of 72)

Reversal Designs Help in Applied Settings (Science Is Lovable 14 of 72)

After considering the ABAB/Reversal type of single–subject designs for basic experimental research in a recent column, this column turns to similar designs for applied research and practice. In applied settings, research considerations differ from laboratory settings, and some compel the adoption of the more complicated “multiple–baseline” single–subject designs that a later column covers.